What intelligence tests might be overlooking when it comes
to autism
By Rose Eveleth
http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=the-hidden-potential-of-autistic-kids
When I was in fifth grade, my brother Alex started correcting my homework. This would not have been weird, except that he was in kindergarten—and autistic. His disorder, characterized by repetitive behaviors and difficulty with social interactions and communication, made it hard for him to listen to his teachers. He was often kicked out of class for not being able to sit for more than a few seconds at a time. Even now, almost 15 years later, he can still barely scratch out his name. But he could look at my page of neatly written words or math problems and pick out which ones were wrong.
When I was in fifth grade, my brother Alex started correcting my homework. This would not have been weird, except that he was in kindergarten—and autistic. His disorder, characterized by repetitive behaviors and difficulty with social interactions and communication, made it hard for him to listen to his teachers. He was often kicked out of class for not being able to sit for more than a few seconds at a time. Even now, almost 15 years later, he can still barely scratch out his name. But he could look at my page of neatly written words or math problems and pick out which ones were wrong.
Many researchers are starting to rethink how much we really
know about autistic
people and their abilities. These researchers are coming to the conclusion
that we might be underestimating what they are capable of contributing to
society. Autism
is a spectrum disease with two very different ends. At one extreme are “high
functioning” people who often hold jobs and keep friends and can get along well
in the world. At the other, "low functioning" side are people who
cannot operate on their own. Many of them are diagnosed with mental retardation
and have to be kept under constant care. But these diagnoses focus on what
autistic people cannot do. Now a growing number of scientists are turning that
around to look at what autistic people are good at.
Researchers have long considered the majority of those
affected by autism to be mentally retarded. Although the numbers cited vary,
they generally fall between 70 to 80 percent of the affected population. But
when Meredyth Edelson, a researcher at Willamette University, went looking for
the source of those statistics, she was surprised that she could not find
anything conclusive. Many of the conclusions were based on intelligence tests
that tend to overestimate disability
in autistic people. "Our knowledge is based on pretty bad data," she
says.
This hidden potential was recently acknowledged
by Laurent Mottron, a psychiatrist at the University of Montreal. In an
article in the November 3 issue of Nature, he recounts his own
experience working with high-functioning autistic people in his lab, which
showed him the power of the autistic brain rather than its limitations. Mottron
concludes that perhaps autism is not really a disease at all—that it is perhaps
just a different way of looking at the world that should be celebrated rather
than viewed as pathology.
Having grown up with two autistic brothers—Alex, four years
younger than I, and Decker, who is eight years younger—Mottron's conclusion
rings true. As I watched them move through the public schools, it became very
clear that there was a big difference between what teachers expected of them
and what they could do. Of course, their autism hindered them in some
ways—which often made school difficult— yet it also seemed to give them fresh
and useful ways of seeing the world—which often don't show up in the standard
intelligence tests.
That is because testing for intelligence in autistic people
is hard. The average person can sit down and take a verbally administered,
timed test without too many problems. But for an autistic person with limited
language capability, who might be easily distracted by sensory information,
this task is very hard. The most commonly administered intelligence test, the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) almost seems designed to flunk
an autistic person: it is a completely verbal, timed test that relies heavily
on cultural and social knowledge. It asks questions like "What is the
thing to do if you find an envelope in the street that is sealed, addressed and
has a new stamp on it?" and "What is the thing to do when you cut
your finger?"
This year Decker was kicked out of a test much like WISC.
Every three years, as he moves through the public school system, his progress
is re-evaluated as a part of his Individualized Education Plan—a set of
guidelines designed to help
people with disabilities reach their educational goal.
This year, as part of the test, the woman delivering the
questions asked him, "You find out someone is getting married. What is an
appropriate question to ask them?"
My brother's answer: "What kind of cake are you
having?"
The proctor shook her head. No, she said, that's not a
correct answer. Try again. He furrowed his brow in the way we have all learned
to be wary of—it is the face that happens before he starts to shut down—and
said, "I don't have another question. That's what I would ask." And
that was that. He would not provide her another question, and she would not
move on without one. He failed that question and never finished the test.
A test does not have to be like this. Other measures, like
Raven's Progressive Matrices or the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI),
avoid these behavioral and language difficulties. They ask children to complete
designs and patterns, with mostly nonverbal instructions. And yet they often
are not used.
The average child will score around the same percentile for
all these tests, both verbal and nonverbal. But an autistic child will not.
Isabelle Soulieres, a researcher at Harvard University, gave a group of
autistics both WISC and the Raven test to measure the difference between the
two groups. Although she expected a difference, she was surprised at just how
big the gap was. On average, autistic students performed 30 percentile points
better on the Raven test than on WISC. Some kids jumped 70 percentile points. "Depending
on which test you use, you get a very different picture of the potential of the
kids," she says. Other studies have confirmed this gap, although they
found a smaller jump between tests.
The “high functioning” autistic children, with the least
severe version of the disability, were not the only ones to score higher.
Soulieres conducted a study recently at a school for autistic children
considered intellectually disabled. Using the Raven test, she found that about
half of them scored in the average range for the general population. "Many
of those who are considered low-functioning—if you give them other intelligence
tests, you will find hidden potential," she says. "They can solve
really complex problems if you give them material that they can optimally process."
What this means, she says, is that schools are
underestimating the abilities of autistic children all across the spectrum. The
widespread use of the WISC in schools has helped set expectations of autistic
kids too low—assuming that they will not be able to learn the same things that
the average child can. Based on the test results, people come to the conclusion
that autistic children cannot learn, when perhaps they do not learn the same
way other people do.
The hidden potential of autistic people seems to fall in
common areas—tasks that involve pattern recognition, logical reasoning and
picking out irregularities in data or arguments. Soulieres describes working
with an autistic woman in her lab who can pick out the slightest flaws in
logic. "At first, we argue with her," Soulieres laughs, "but
almost each time, she's right, and we're wrong."
Recognizing these
talents, rather than pushing them aside to focus on the drawbacks of
autism, could benefit not just autistic people, but everyone else as well.
Mottron chronicles how much better his science got by working with his autistic
lab partner. I got far higher marks on my homework than I would have without
Alex, even though his corrections were sometimes infuriating. And many think
their potential extends beyond science to all professions, if given the right
chances.
Just because a test says someone has potential, that does
not mean it is easy to realize. My brother Decker’s teachers are convinced—and
the tests confirm—that he has hidden potential. But in class, he often falls
behind when trying to listen to instructions and gets frustrated when trying to
catch up. "It doesn't mean that it's easy for them in everyday life, or
that it's easy for their parents or teachers," Soulieres says. "But
it shows that they have this reasoning potential, and maybe we have to start
teaching them differently and stop making the assumption that they won't
learn."
More and more people are starting to wonder what gems might
lie hidden in the autistic brain.
And if my brothers are any indication, if we keep looking, we will find them.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar